3.3 Marimbas and xylophones

PREVIOUS SECTION

Now we know how ideal beams behave we are ready to look at their serious musical relatives, the xylophone and marimba. Each note of these instruments has a bar of wood or metal, supported in some way at points roughly a quarter of the way in from each end. When you tap the bar, you hear a sound that should be perceived as having a definite musical pitch. The bars for different notes have different sizes: longer bars for the lower notes, shorter ones for higher notes.

Toy xylophones use uniform metal bars very much like the idealised theory we saw in section 3.2. Is a toy xylophone “musical”? Well, up to a point. The notes of these toys have rectangular metal bars, which will fall somewhere between the idealised “beam” and “plate”. The various sound examples in the previous section showed that a plate can easily become extremely unmusical, but a beam is better. The sound of both was improved by using a softer hammer. The explanation of all this is that none of the overtone frequencies are “tuned” to be in harmonic relations with each other, so their sounds tend to detract from any sense of definite pitch based on the fundamental.

Actually, a perceived pitch need not be based on a fundamental frequency which is physically present in the sound. A mixture of frequencies with ratios 2:3:4, for example, can often produce a sensation of a pitched note at the frequency that would have been “1” in this series. This phenomenon is known by several names: the “missing fundamental”, or “residue pitch”, for example. We will meet an in the next section, when we look at church bells.

Getting back to the sound of a vibrating beam, there are two things we can do to encourage a listener to hear a definite pitch. The simple approach is to arrange things so that one overtone, usually the fundamental, is much louder than any of the overtones. This can be done by using a soft hammer, but the grown-up xylophone and marimba enhance this effect by using resonators. As can be seen in Fig. 1, a suitably tuned tube is placed under each bar, to enhance the sound radiated at the fundamental frequency. We will see in section 4.3 how such resonators work. You can directly hear the effect of these resonators in the vibraphone, which has a set of rotating vanes in the tubes so that the resonant effect is modulated in and out in a regular way.

There is a second approach to making the sound more “musical”: the instrument maker can do some simple vibration engineering to make marimba bars just a little more harmonic. They do this by removing wood from the underside of the bars so that the thickness varies along the length of the bar: see some examples in Fig. 2. Actually, the maker has several reasons for doing this “undercutting” of the bars. Uppermost in their mind, probably, is simply that they need to tune each bar to the precise pitch called for by each separate note. Second, especially for the lower notes on a marimba, vigorous undercutting allows you to get away with a shorter bar than you would otherwise need, with obvious advantages in weight, portability and usage of expensive raw material.

Figure 2. A selection of marimba bars, showing the undercutting for tuning.

But there is a third thing a skilled maker can do by careful undercutting: they can bring the ratio of the first two natural frequencies of the bar close to a whole number. Recall that in the ideal beam this ratio was 2.76. We will see shortly that symmetrical undercutting around the middle of the bar allows you make this ratio bigger, but not easily to make it smaller. So well-tuned marimba and xylophone bars have this ratio adjusted to be close to 3 or, much more commonly, 4. The examples shown in Fig. 2 all have enough undercutting to produce 4:1 tuning.

So how do the sounds compare, from these different options? Sound 1 below is a repeat of Sound 5 from section 3.2, with the frequency ratios of an ideal beam, and modal Q-factors of 100. Sound 2 is the result of rounding all these ratios to the nearest whole number, so that all overtones are perfect harmonics. Sound 3 is based on measured frequency ratios from a xylophone bar in which the second mode was tuned to three times the fundamental, and Sound 4 is a similar thing but based on measured frequency ratios from a marimba bar in which the second mode was tuned to four times the fundamental. The ratios for the xylophone bar are 1.00, 3.00, 6.16, 10.29, 14.01, 19.66, 24.02. For the marimba bar, they are 1.00, 3.92, 9.24, 16.27, 24.22, 33.54, 42.97. In both cases, the measured frequency ratios do not seem to be deliberately tuned beyond the second mode.

To my ear, the two cases based on measured frequencies sound better than Sound 2 with the “perfect” harmonics. The non-harmonic higher overtones seem to give an interesting percussive edge to the sound which is lacking in Sound 2. But I hear something else interesting: when comparing Sound 3 with Sound 4 in rapid succession, the actual pitches of the two scales can sound different. But the fundamental frequencies are exactly the same in both cases, and the second mode is tuned to a near-harmonic in both cases. My brain seems to be responding, in some way, to the 4:3 ratio of the two second frequencies, plus the fact that all the higher overtones tend to follow in a similar ratio. Recall from section 2.3 that 4:3 is the frequency ratio for an interval of a fourth in musical language, and this is indeed the “pitch” difference that I sometimes hear when comparing these two examples.

Sound 1. Synthesised scale based on frequencies of an ideal beam, with Q=100;
Sound 2. Synthesised scale based on frequencies of an ideal beam rounded to the nearest whole number, with Q=100;
Sound 3. Synthesised scale based on measured frequency ratios from a xylophone bar, with Q=100;
Sound 4. Synthesised scale based on measured frequency ratios from a marimba bar, with Q=100;

To begin to understand how the tuning process is done, we can use a method based on Rayleigh’s principle, introduced in the previous section, to give a graphical representation of what a maker can expect to do by adjusting the pattern of thickness. As explained in the next link, from the mode shapes (as plotted in Fig. 1 in the previous section) we can compute a function $G(x)$ which tells you the sensitivity of the natural frequency of that mode to any small change in the thickness pattern. Examples for the first three modes of a uniform beam are plotted in the red lines in Fig. 3. The dashed blue lines show the corresponding mode shapes. If a little wood is removed in a particular area of the beam, this function tells you how much that mode frequency will change. Removing wood where $G$ is negative will make the frequency go down; removing wood where $G$ is positive will make it go up.

SEE MORE DETAIL

Figure 3. Sensitivity of modal frequency to thickness adjustment, for the first three modes of a free-free beam. The mode shape is shown as a blue dashed line, the sensitivity function $G(x)$ as a red line.

For the lowest mode, the function $G$ looks rather like the mode shape itself. It is easy to understand why the curve has to be roughly this shape. Recall that the vibration frequency is determined by the balance between bending stiffness and inertia. Removing wood near the ends, where $G$ is positive, means that you reduce the inertia. You also reduce the bending stiffness, but since there is no bending of the beam near a free end of the beam, this doesn’t matter. So the frequency goes up, because the dominant effect is a reduction of inertia. This mode has its maximum bending in the centre. Removing wood there will reduce the bending stiffness, and this effect will outweigh the reduction of inertia (mainly because bending stiffness is proportional to $h^3$ but inertia is only proportional to $h$, so bending stiffness is more sensitively affected by a thickness reduction). So the frequency goes down.

The other plots in Fig. 3 show that the function $G$ does not usually look at all like the mode shape: the first mode is an exception. For the second and third modes, the only place $G$ goes positive is near the ends. Everywhere else $G \le 0$ so that wood removal will reduce the frequency. But mode 2 has $G=0$ at the centre, so removing wood near that point will have very little effect on this mode frequency, whereas we have already seen that it will reduce the frequency of mode 1. That is the main reason that normal undercutting will increase the frequency ratio between modes 1 and 2.

The sensitivity curves plotted here are only strictly relevant to a small change in thickness, starting from a uniform beam. The amount of undercutting on normal marimba bars is far too large for these plots to tell the whole story of the tuning process. However, the same approach can be applied for each step of the undercutting process: given the mode shapes at a given stage, plots like these could be generated to guide the next stage. In practice, although the total undercutting can be quite drastic, the general character of the mode shapes does not change very much, and so the sensitivity functions remain roughly the same shape throughout. Some examples can be seen in Fletcher and Rossing [1]: see the chapter “Mallet percussion instruments”.

NEXT SECTION


[1] Neville H Fletcher and Thomas D Rossing; “The physics of musical instruments”, Springer-Verlag (Second edition 1998)