7.5 Tonal adjustment in the violin: the bridge

PREVIOUS SECTION

Musical instruments differ in the extent to which the sound can be fine-tuned after the instrument maker has finished their job. In a guitar with a glued-in bridge, there is relatively little that can be done beyond choosing strings. On the other hand, we saw (and heard) back in section 5.5 that some features of a banjo can be modified by the player, to tailor the sound to what they prefer — for example the head tension can be changed, and different designs of bridge can be used. Indeed, any stringed instrument with a tailpiece and a “floating” bridge offers the possibility of bridge modification. In a piano, the bridge, soundboard structure and string choice are all fixed unless a major refurbishment is done — but there is scope for fine adjustments to the action and to the stiffness of the felt that lines the hammers. A skilled piano technician can make significant changes to the sound and feel of an instrument, as shown in graphic detail in the film “Pianomania”. We will look at some of the subtleties of piano hammers in section 12.2.

In this section, we will look at the violin family. There are several features of a violin or cello that can be adjusted, to the extent that “tone adjustment” is part of the job description of many violin makers, with some of them specialising in it to the extent that players may bring an instrument across the world to be tweaked. The main ingredients of this tonal adjustment involve the bridge and the soundpost: small changes to both can have surprisingly significant effects, and it is this sensitivity that we will explore in this section (for the bridge) and the following section (for the soundpost).

A. Modelling the bridge

We start with the bridge. We have already said a bit about the dynamics of a violin bridge back in section 5.3, but now we need to go into more detail. Figure 1 shows a typical violin bridge. When the strings are bowed, they apply forces at the corresponding string notches at the top of the bridge. Those forces are predominantly in the plane of the bridge and in the direction of bowing, as indicated by the red arrows. The body of the violin is driven by the forces generated at the bridge feet, indicated schematically by the green arrows. The feet are small, so we can think of these forces as being applied to the body at two points.

Figure 1. A violin bridge, showing the main direction of force applied at each string notch when the string is bowed, and the forces applied to the violin body by the bridge feet.

To understand the role of the bridge, we need to know two things. For the influence on the sound of the violin, we need to know how a force in some specified direction on the top of the bridge is converted into the two forces through the feet. These foot forces will drive body vibration and hence sound radiation, in a way that will not change if the bridge is altered. To understand the influence back on the bowed string itself, to determine “playability”, we need to know the motion induced at the string notch, described by a frequency response function we have met earlier, the bridge admittance.

As we have already seen, there are two types of bridge deformation we need to consider for a violin. There may be others, of course, but these two are the most studied and probably the most important. They are most easily visualised by thinking of the bridge clamped by its feet in a heavy vice. The bridge would then have two in-plane vibration modes within the frequency range of interest: a rocking mode usually around 3 kHz and a bouncing mode usually around 6 kHz. We can idealise these as simple mass-spring oscillators. Figure 2 shows the idealisation of the rocking mode. A rigid base carries the two feet, and the rocking motion is visualised as a weighted bar, pivoted to the base and controlled by a torsional spring (like an old-fashioned clock spring). The rocking frequency is determined by the ratio of this spring stiffness to the moment of inertia of the weighted bar.

Figure 2. Schematic model of the rocking mode of a violin bridge, excited by a transverse force at the top. A rigid base is joined to a weighted rod that can pivot about the red dot. A torsion spring (shown in red) restrains this motion, and the combination of the spring stiffness and the moment of inertia of the rod determines the rocking frequency when the bridge feet are fixed.

The corresponding idealised version of the bouncing mode is shown in Fig. 3. This time, a mass is supported on a linear spring, connected to a rigid base similar to the one in Fig. 2. The bouncing frequency would be governed by the ratio of the spring stiffness to the mass. If we ignore the slight asymmetry of the bridge shape, a transverse force at the centre of the bridge would excite only the rocking mode, while a vertical force would excite only the bouncing mode. These forces are indicated by blue arrows in the respective figures.

Figure 3. Schematic model of the bouncing mode of a violin bridge, excited by a vertical force at the top. A mass is connected to the rigid base by a linear spring. The combination of the spring stiffness and the mass determines the bouncing frequency when the bridge feet are fixed.

Both these idealised models are straightforward to analyse: the details are given in the next link. However, these are by no means the end of the story: several factors have been ignored, and it is possible to construct a succession of more complicated models to bring these factors progressively into play. Needless to say, the gain in accuracy is offset by an increase in complexity and a reduction in immediate intuitive appeal: choosing the “best” model is a matter of balancing conflicting requirements. The link gives details of the first step in building a more complete model. It combines the two simple models into a single analysis which removes some approximations made in the simplified models, and also allows us to use excitation at the various string positions and angles shown in Fig. 1, rather than just the ideal cases of excitation at the bridge centre point as in Figs. 2 and 3.

SEE MORE DETAIL

We will return later to discuss further complications of the model (see subsection E and its associated side link). One aspect suggested by Figs. 1—3 is that the forces on the violin body have only been considered in the direction normal to the surface of the top plate. Measurements shown in section 7.5.3 suggest that this is indeed the most important component of force, but that there is also a side-to-side component which has a smaller effect which can nevertheless be significant.

We are not, of course, really interested in the case with the bridge feet clamped in a vice: we want the feet to be resting on the body of a violin. For the purposes of the approximate model described in the link, we can capture the relevant behaviour of that violin body by a set of frequency response functions, which can be measured on a violin with its bridge removed as illustrated in Fig. 4. Two small accelerometers have been fixed to the positions where the bridge feet will sit, then a small impulse hammer is used to tap on one or the other of these accelerometers. Additional detail relating to this measurement is given in the next link: there are some important subtleties.

SEE MORE DETAIL

Figure 4. Measuring the response of a violin body at the positions of the bridge feet, using two small accelerometers and a miniature impulse hammer.

The measurement gives four frequency response functions, but two of them should be exactly the same by virtue of a general property of vibrating systems called reciprocity. For any linear vibrating system, if you tap at one position and measure at a second position, you should get exactly the same response if you swap the tapping and measuring positions. A typical set of measurements is shown in Fig. 5. The red and blue curves show the driving-point admittances at the two foot positions, while the black and green curves show the two cross terms. These are indeed very similar, as reciprocity requires. The red and blue curves illustrate a pattern which is common to all normal violins. At low frequencies, the red curve for the bass bridge foot is higher than the blue curve for the treble foot. But at high frequencies this is reversed, with the blue curve lying higher than the red one.

Figure 5. A typical set of measured body response admittances. The red line shows the driving-point admittance at the bass bridge foot position, the blue line shows the corresponding result at the treble foot position. The black and green dashed lines show the two cross terms. They can be seen to be almost equal, as required by reciprocity.

B. Testing the models

Armed with these results for the body response, we can calculate several interesting things from the two idealised bridge models shown in Figs. 2 and 3 (and from the combined and extended model described in the first side link). We can predict the bridge admittance when the bridge feet are resting on the violin, and also the response at the bridge feet to excitation by an impulse hammer or by string forces at the top of the bridge. We can go further: we can try to find the parameter values of the models (mass, spring stiffness, moment of inertia) by best-fitting the predicted results to the actual measured bridge admittance. We can then do “virtual bridge adjustment”: the model can be used to explore what would happen to the bridge admittance or the bridge-foot forces if those masses or stiffnesses were varied.

The first step is to find out if the models work well enough to be useful in this way: can they give a convincing prediction of measured bridge admittance, with plausible values for the model parameters? The rocking model sketched in Fig. 2 has three parameters: the stiffness of the torsional spring, the moment of inertia of the weighted bar (representing the top part of the bridge), and the distance from the pivot point to the bridge top. To use the combined model, we also need to include values for the two parameters of the bouncing model sketched in Fig. 3: spring stiffness and mass. Measurements of the rocking admittance and the vertical admittance at the bridge centre can be used to determine these parameters. We will concentrate mainly on the rocking case, which is of most direct interest to normal violin playing, but the same procedures have also been applied to the vertical admittance to complete the set of estimated parameter values: we will see an example later.

Choosing particular values (of three parameters for the simplified model or five parameters for the combined model), the rocking admittance can be calculated. This can be compared with the measured version, and we can invent a metric to put a number on how different they are. For the results to be shown here, that metric is inspired by the plot we normally use to assess by eye how good the match might be. The two frequency responses are plotted on a logarithmic frequency scale, over a range from 180 Hz (just below the lowest note of a violin) to 8 kHz (high enough to capture the two formant features, “hills”, that we are interested in). Amplitude is plotted on a decibel scale. Our metric is then the root-mean-square (“RMS”) difference between these two decibel plots, a commonly-used measure. Note that because the decibel difference is first squared, it doesn’t matter which curve is higher and which is lower. All that matters is the vertical distance between the two curves.

We can now run the calculation with a grid of values of the three parameters, and see how the metric varies. The combination that gives the lowest value is our best match, at least according to this particular metric. The best match can be fine-tuned by running an optimisation routine in the computer, using the combined bridge model to fit all 5 parameters for the two bridge modes. An example is shown in Figs. 6 , 7and 8. Figure 6 shows a 3D plot of the metric, for a wide range of values for the moment of inertia, the rocking resonance frequency (which then determines the torsional spring stiffness), and the effective height as indicated in Fig. 2. Figure 7 shows a contour map on a horizontal cross-section at the best-fitted height, 19 mm. The contours are spaced 0.5 dB apart, starting from the lowest value, which in this case was an RMS difference of 3.4 dB. That minimum is marked by a red star, corresponding to a rocking frequency of 2.9 kHz and a moment of inertia value of $4.3 \times 10^{-7}\ \mathrm{kg~m}^2$. To see what this RMS difference means, Fig. 8 shows the measured and predicted admittances. It can be seen that the two curves are gratifyingly close.

Figure 6. 3D plot to show how the simulated bridge admittance differs from the measured one, as the clamped-foot resonance frequency, the moment of inertia and the effective height for the rocking resonance are varied over a wide range. The metric for goodness of fit is the RMS difference between the two amplitude spectra, on a logarithmic frequency scale. Red points indicate values of that metric less than 3.5 dB, blue points indicate less than 4 dB, black points indicate less that 4.5 dB. The red circle indicates the best-fit position.
Figure 7. Contour map to show how the simulated bridge admittance differs from the measured one, as the clamped-foot resonance frequency and the moment of inertia are varied over a wide range. This plot is a horizontal cross-section of Fig. 6 through the best-fit point at height 19 mm. Contours are at 0.5 dB spacing, and the best fit is marked by the red star.
Figure 8. The measured bridge admittance (red curve) and the best-fitted version from the extended model (blue curve) at the minimum point of the contour plot in Fig. 7.

You may have noticed something in the animation of Fig. 6: the cloud of points makes a “sausage” shape, but this sausage has a very pronounced sideways tilt. This tells us that there is a strong correlation between the moment of inertia and the effective rocking length. We can shed some light on this by plotting the same results in a different way, shown in Fig. 9. The moment of inertia can be expressed in terms of an effective point mass, positioned at the top centre of the bridge. This mass depends on both the moment of inertia and the rocking length, and when the results are plotted as a function of this effective mass, the resulting “sausage” is now nearly vertical. This effective mass is strictly a fictitious quantity (you couldn’t measure it by weighing any particular portion of the bridge), but the vertical sausage means that it is in fact the quantity that is determined most clearly by the requirement of matching the measured admittance. This observation will be explained in the final side link, which comes at the very end of this section.

Figure 9. The same data as in Fig. 6, but now plotted with effective mass (in grams) in place of moment of inertia on one of the three axes.

Next, we need to check that if the same bridge is installed on a different violin, the fitted bridge parameters remain essentially the same even though the admittance will be different. This test is difficult to do with normal violin bridges, because the feet are carefully carved to fit the arching profile of each individual instrument, and would not be expected to give a proper fit on a different one. However, there is a type of commercially-available violin bridge (aimed presumably at the amateur market) in which the feet can swivel so that there is a much better chance of fitting to two different instruments. These bridges are ideal for the test we want to perform.

Figure 10 shows three of these bridges, modified to give very different total masses so that we can also get an impression of how much difference the bridge might make to the admittance. The middle one of the three bridges is fairly normal, with a mass of 2.4 g. The right-hand one has had some “extraneous” wood removed, and it is also a lot thinner. The result is a mass of only 1.5 g. The left-hand bridge has been left thick, and in an effort to raise the frequencies of the rocking and bouncing modes the cutouts have been filled with epoxy. The result is a total mass of 3.3 g.

Figure 10. Three contrasting bridges, with adjustable feet so that they can be installed on more than one violin for testing purposes.

Figures 11, 12 and 13 show the result of installing these bridges in turn on the same violin used for the earlier tests. Each figure shows the contour map of the RMS difference metric, and also shows the best-fitted admittance compared to the corresponding measurement. The same vertical scale is used for all the admittance plots, and if you compare them carefully you can see clear differences. Figure 11 shows the lightest bridge, which produces the lowest fitted value of the moment of inertia, and gives the highest admittance levels around the “hill” in the 2—4 kHz range. Figure 12 shows the intermediate bridge. It has a higher moment of inertia, and the admittance in the hill region is lower.

Figure 13 shows the heaviest bridge. The admittance is lower still in the hill region, and we see something interesting in the contour plot. Instead of a clear-cut minimum marked by a red star in the middle of the plot, the lowest contour line runs off the top right corner of the diagram and the red star is on the boundary of the plotted region. There is no clear prediction of a rocking resonance frequency — which is not surprising since this filled-in bridge is not really able to “bend at the waist” in the classic rocking resonance. The contour map is telling us that for a bridge like this, the admittance is very insensitive to the stiffness of the torsion spring, provided that stiffness is high enough. This is exactly what we should have expected: with a very stiff bridge, the admittance is dominated by the effective rocking stiffness of the “island” area of the violin top, where the bridge sits. If that island stiffness is much lower than the bridge stiffness, the bridge is simply carried along rigidly during the rocking motion, and the exact value of its stiffness makes little difference. The same description applies, to a lesser extent, to Fig. 12.

The same three bridges were then installed in turn on a different violin. The corresponding results are shown in Figs. 14, 15 and 16. The detailed admittance plots are significantly different, most obviously at low frequency where the pattern of “signature mode” peaks is quite different. However, the trends across the three bridges are the same, and the contour maps are recognisably similar to the corresponding ones in Figs. 11, 12 and 13. In particular, Figs. 14 and 15 show the innermost contours in very similar places to those in Figs. 11 and 12. Figure 16 shows a red star on the right-hand edge, in a similar way to Fig. 13. The conclusion is just as we would hope: the procedure of best-fitting the theoretical model gives encouragingly robust predictions of the bridge properties, regardless of which violin is used.

It is useful to see examples of the corresponding fitting process applied to the vertical admittance, measured by tapping and observing in the vertical direction at the centre of the bridge’s top curve, between the D and A strings of the violin. Figure 17 shows the results, for the middle one of the three bridges on the original violin: the same combination as in Fig. 12. The admittance is plotted on the same vertical scale as the earlier ones, and it is immediately apparent that the values are significantly lower than in the rocking case. As we should have expected, the admittance in the right-hand plot of Fig. 17 shows a new “hill” around 5 kHz, based on the bouncing resonance of the bridge and the island area of the violin.

Figure 18 shows the corresponding results for the same bridge on the other violin, the same combination of instrument and bridge shown in Fig. 15. It shows similarly low values to Fig. 17, and a slightly less obvious hill formant around 5 kHz. The comparison of the two contour maps is interesting. Both of them show a rather featureless blank area in the upper right. The interpretation is similar to the discussion of the rocking behaviour of the heaviest bridge: when it comes to bouncing, this less heavy bridge is sufficiently stiff compared to the island area of the violin that the admittance is insensitive to the exact spring stiffness. Under these circumstances we cannot expect to obtain accurate values of the bridge’s parameters by the fitting process — but that doesn’t matter very much because of the insensitivity.

Figures 17 and 18 highlight an aspect of the best-fitting process that I have glossed over in the account so far. For reasons that will be become clear shortly, it is best to fit the combined model which takes account of both bridge resonances and involves all 5 parameters. All the fits that have been shown have in fact made use of this combined model, and both the measured bridge admittances (rocking and vertical). The RMS difference metrics for these two admittances have simply been added together to give a combined metric, and this has been minimised to determine all 5 parameter values.

C. Virtual bridge adjustment

We now have a reasonable level of confidence in the theoretical model, and we can use it to explore a question of great interest to violin makers. Starting from a given bridge on a given violin, how much difference could be made to the admittance or the sound of the instrument, by adjusting the bridge within the bounds of feasibility? Figure 19 gives an illustration of one way to address that question. We can take the violin/bridge combination studied in Figs. 5–8, and vary the rocking resonance frequency and the moment of inertia by a certain factor upwards or downwards from the nominal fitted values. For this figure, the chosen factor is 30%: admittedly quite a large variation, but this makes it easier to see the trends.

Figure 19. Example of “virtual bridge adjustment”, showing a set of rocking bridge admittance curves with their associated “skeleton curves”. The central plot is the nominal admittance as in Fig. 8, fitted to a measurement. In the left and right columns (with different plot colours) the rigid-foot rocking frequency has been decreased and increased by 30%, respectively. In the upper and lower rows (plotted with different line types), the moment of inertia has been decreased and increased by 30%, respectively.

The central plot shows the nominal case, the same admittance as the red curve in Fig. 8. Superimposed on this is a “skeleton curve”: we introduced this idea back in section 5.3 when we first met the bridge hill. By averaging the admittance over a bandwidth wide enough to contain several resonances, we can obtain a curve which tracks the mean level of the decibel plot. This makes the “hill” more clear by smoothing out the detailed peaks and dips caused by the individual body resonances of the violin. Surrounding this plot are 8 others. In the left-hand column, the clamped-foot bridge rocking resonance frequency has been reduced by 30%, while in the right-hand column it has been increased by 30%. In the top row the moment of inertia has been reduced by 30%, while in the bottom row it has been increased by 30%. All other parameters are kept fixed.

Figure 20. The set of skeleton curves from Fig. 19, all superimposed and plotted on an expanded vertical scale. The line colours and types are the same as in Fig. 19: the solid red curve relates to the nominal admittance, different colours denote different values of the rocking frequency, and different line types denote different values of the moment of inertia.

All the plots use the same vertical scale, but even so it is a little hard to focus on the comparison between any two particular cases. To make such comparisons easier, Fig. 20 collects the 9 skeleton curves together in a single plot. The line colours and types match those used in Fig. 19: the solid red line is the reference case, changes in colour indicate variations in rocking frequency, changes in line type indicate variations in moment of inertia. This combined plot makes it clear that if variations as large as 30% in these bridge parameters could be made, very significant changes in admittance should result: for example, the height of the “hill” peak varies over a range bigger than 10 dB. The implication is that even with more modest changes to the bridge parameters, variations of a few dB should be possible.

So far, we have only talked about the influence of bridge properties on admittance. But of course we are also interested in the influence on the sound of the violin when played. We argued at the start of this section that the influence of bridge variations on radiated sound should be captured by a pair of transfer functions, relating the force at the two bridge feet to the force applied at the top of the bridge (by the vibrating strings or by an impulse hammer used for testing).

We can calculate these two transfer functions from a fitted theoretical model: but now it is important to use the combined and extended model, rather than the simplified ones. As explained in detail in the first side link above, both simplified models predict that the forces at the two feet will be equal in magnitude (in phase for bouncing, in opposite phases for rocking), but the combined model reveals that it is not so simple. Figure 21 shows an example, using the fitted model which produced the admittance shown in the blue curve of Fig. 8. The force at the bass foot of the bridge is shown in red, while the force at the treble foot is shown in blue. In the middle of the plotted frequency range, the two are rather similar; but at high and low frequencies the red curve lies significantly above the blue curve. This particular plot uses an input force to match the way the admittance was measured: the hammer tap was applied to the bass corner of the bridge, oriented parallel to the bowing direction on the nearby G string.

Figure 21. For the original violin and bridge, giving the bridge admittance shown in the blue curve in Fig. 8, this plot shows the predicted transfer functions relating the input force to the force at the bass foot of the bridge (red) and the treble foot of the bridge (blue). The input force is applied at the G corner of the bridge, in the direction parallel to the bowing angle on the G string, as it was in the original admittance measurement.

We can use the extended model to investigate how the various transfer functions vary between the different string notches, each with a corresponding main bowing direction (tangential to the top curve of the bridge, as indicated back in Fig. 1). Figure 22 shows the answer for the bridge admittance. The four curves correspond to the four strings (G in red, D in blue, A in green, E in black). The four admittances are remarkably similar, although there are differences amounting to a few dB in some peak heights.

Figure 22. The bridge admittance in the bowing direction at the four string notches: G string (red), D string (blue), A string (green) and E string (black).

But when we look at the corresponding transfer function to forces at the feet, shown in Fig. 23, there are much more obvious differences. For the G string (top left plot), the red curve for the bass foot lies above the blue curve for the treble foot, at both high and low frequencies. But this balance changes as we move across the strings, and by the time we reach the E string (bottom right plot) the pattern is reversed. Since the sound radiation behaviour from forces at the two feet will be different, this may translate into systematic differences of sound across the four strings.

Figure 23. The transfer functions to force at the bridge feet, in the same format as Fig. 21, for force applied in the bowing direction at the four string notches: G and D strings in the top row, A and E strings in the bottom row. Force at the bass foot of the bridge is in red, and force at the treble foot of the bridge is in blue.

It is interesting to repeat the “virtual bridge adjustment” with these force transfer functions. Figure 24 shows an example, for forcing at the G string notch. The nine cases are laid out in the same way as in Fig. 18, with $\pm30\%$ variations of rocking resonance frequency across the columns and $\pm30\%$ variations in moment of inertia across the rows. In each small plot, the transfer function to the bass foot is plotted in red (along with its corresponding “skeleton curve”), while the corresponding plots for the treble foot are shown in blue.

Figure 24. Variation of the foot force transfer functions for the same set of virtual bridge adjustments as in Fig. 19. In each case the red curve relates to the bass bridge foot, the blue curve to the treble foot. Skeleton curves have also been shown in each case.

Differences can be seen, but to make comparisons easier the skeleton curves for all the cases have been collected together: Fig. 25 shows the results for the bass foot force, while Fig. 26 shows the corresponding results for the treble foot force. For these two plots, the code of colours and line types from Fig. 20 has been adopted so that these figures are directly comparable. Colours indicate variations in rocking frequency, line types indicate variations in moment of inertia. The pattern of variation is different in the two cases, and both are different from the pattern shown by admittance in Fig. 20.

Figure 25. The collection of skeleton curves from Fig. 24, for the bass bridge foot. The same set of colours and line types have been used as in Fig. 20 to distinguish the different bridge modifications.
Figure 26. The collection of skeleton curves from Fig. 24, for the treble bridge foot. The same set of colours and line types have been used as in Figs. 20 and 25 to distinguish the different bridge modifications.

D. Making sounds

The obvious next step is to listen to these effects of bridge adjustment. We have already seen how this might be done, back in section 6.5: we can record the force waveform at the bridge when a violinist plays a suitable passage, then use it as input to the bridge model that we have developed here. But before we can put this into practice, we need one more measurement on the test violin. The plots in Figs. 23 and 24 show how the force at the string notch is converted to forces at the two bridge feet. We now need to know the transfer functions to convert those foot forces into sound pressure at a chosen microphone position. These are straightforward to measure: with the violin in the state shown in Fig. 4, with the bridge removed, we tap at the bridge-foot positions with the impulse hammer, and record the resulting measured sound.

Figure 27 shows an example of the results. The violin is the same one used for most of the results shown in this section — it is an amateur-made instrument by the author, from 1990. The sound measurements were made in a fairly dead domestic space, with a microphone placed 0.4 m from the centre of the violin in the plane of the bridge, at an angle of 30$^\circ$ towards the bass side from the front-facing axis through the centre of the bridge. The radiated sound from tapping at the bass bridge foot is shown in red, from the treble foot in blue.

Figure 27. Sound pressure measured at a particular microphone position in response to a tap at the bass bridge foot position (red) and the treble bridge foot position (blue)

Both curves show the general features we would expect from a microphone test. At low frequency there are strong, well-separated peaks corresponding to the “signature modes” of the violin body. However, at higher frequency the curves look more complicated than what would be obtained from a mechanical measurement like the admittances in Fig. 5. This is the inevitable result of the complications of sound radiation and of interaction with the acoustics of the measurement space — these issues will be discussed in some detail in section 10.4(D). Underneath the “fuzziness” of the curves, a trend is clear. The sound coming from the bass bridge foot (red curve) is significantly stronger at low frequency than the sound from the treble foot (blue curve). Above about 1 kHz the two curves have comparable levels, and in some frequency ranges the blue curve rises above the red curve.

Having raised the issue of the complicating effects of sound radiation and room acoustics, it is interesting to ask how much the results would be affected by a different choice of microphone position. Figure 28 gives a partial answer. The left-hand plot shows the red curve from Fig. 27, compared to the cloud of results obtained by making the same measurement at a circle of 12 microphone positions, equally spaced around a circle in the plane of the bridge. The right-hand plot shows the same comparison for tapping at the treble foot: the red curve now corresponds to the blue curve in Fig. 27.

Several conclusions can be drawn from these plots. Below about 1 kHz, the signature mode peaks have very similar heights for all microphone positions. This behaviour is as we should expect from the discussion in section 4.3. At these low frequencies the wavelength of sound is large compared to the size of the violin body, so the sound radiation is dominated by a monopole pattern, the same in all directions. Notice that the difference of level between the bass foot results and the treble foot results is seen for every microphone position.

At higher frequency, the cloud of blue curves covers a wide range. Moving the microphone position can make a big difference to the measured sound, and the details are different for excitation at the bass and treble feet. There is simply no such thing as a “best” microphone position for the purpose of generating synthesised sounds. However, the main aim here is to illustrate the auditory effects of bridge adjustment, and for that purpose all that matters is that the microphone transfer functions are always the same.

We are now ready to listen to some sounds. The starting point is a short extract from the Tchaikowsky violin concerto, played only on the G string and recorded using a force sensor in the G-string notch (see Fig. 1 of section 6.5 for an image of the violin bridge equipped with suitable force sensors). Sound 1 is that recorded bridge force signal. Now we can filter that signal using the force transfer functions shown in Fig. 21, and the radiated sound transfer functions shown in Fig. 27. We do this separately for the bass and treble feet, then add the two results together to simulate the total sound that should be measured at the chosen microphone position. Sound 2 is the result for the treble foot only, Sound 3 is the corresponding result for the bass foot, and Sound 4 is the final combined signal.

Sound 1. The recorded bridge force signal. The extract is a passage from the Tchaikowsky violin concerto, played by Keir GoGwilt
Sound 2. The bridge force from Sound 1, filtered to give the predicted sound radiated from the treble foot of the test violin to a particular microphone position.
Sound 3. The bridge force from Sound 1, filtered to give the predicted sound radiated from the bass foot of the test violin to a particular microphone position.
Sound 4. The sum of Sounds 2 and 3, the combined predicted sound of the test violin at the chosen microphone position

All four of these sounds are distinctly different. There is a strong contrast between the results for the bass and treble feet: satisfyingly, the bass foot gives a more “bassy” sound, and the treble foot a more tinny “treble” sound. This difference of sound is mainly down to the systematic low-frequency difference between the two sound radiation curves in Fig. 27: Fig. 21 shows that the predicted forces at the two feet are rather similar. To my ears at least, the combined Sound 4 does indeed sound more violin-like than either of Sounds 2 and 3 taken separately.

Now we are ready to listen to some examples of virtual bridge adjustment. Be warned that the sounds will all be fairly similar: you may need headphones or good audio reproduction to hear the differences. Figure 29 is a copy of Fig. 24, annotated with markers to show the cases to be demonstrated. The datum case is marked by a gray circle, and appears in Sound 5. This is actually the same as Sound 4, but repeated here for convenience of comparing with the other cases. The light red circle marks the case with the rocking frequency reduced by 30%, with the result in Sound 6. The darker red circle has the rocking frequency increased by 30%, with the result in Sound 7. The lighter blue circle marks the case with the moment of inertia reduced by 30% (but keeping the rocking frequency the same as the datum case), with the result in Sound 8. The darker blue circle has the moment of inertia increased by 30%, with the result in Sound 9.

Figure 29. A copy of Fig. 24, annotated to mark the cases that appear in Sounds 5–9.
Sound 5. The datum case for the following examples of virtual bridge adjustment — it is the same as Sound 4, and is marked by a gray circle in Fig. 29.
Sound 6. The modified version of Sound 5 when the rocking frequency is reduced by 30%. It is marked by a pale red circle in Fig. 29.
Sound 7. The modified version of Sound 5 when the rocking frequency is increased by 30%. It is marked by a dark red circle in Fig. 29.
Sound 8. The modified version of Sound 5 when the rocking rocking moment of inertia is reduced by 30%. It is marked by a pale blue circle in Fig. 29.
Sound 9. The modified version of Sound 5 when the rocking rocking moment of inertia is increased by 30%. It is marked by a dark blue circle in Fig. 29.

To see what lies behind these relatively subtle differences in sound, we can plot the decibel difference between each modified case and the datum case. Figure 30 shows an example, for variation in rocking frequency (keeping all the other model parameters fixed). The red curve shows the difference between Sound 6 and Sound 5, the blue curve shows the difference between Sound 7 and Sound 5. Up to about 1 kHz the differences are very small, but at higher frequencies there are differences of up to about 6 dB. Reducing the rocking frequency increases the sound output in the range 1—3 kHz, and decreases it in the range 4—8 kHz. Increasing the rocking frequency gives a pattern which is the other way round, but the blue curve is not an exact mirror image of the red curve.

Figure 30. Differences in frequency spectrum resulting from adjustments of the bridge rocking frequency. The red curve shows the decibel difference between Sounds 6 and 5, for a 30% reduction in rocking frequency. The blue curve shows the difference between Sounds 7 and 5, for a 30% increase in rocking frequency.

Figures 31 and 32 show corresponding plots for varying the moment of inertia (corresponding to Sounds 8 and 9), and for varying the effective rocking length. In both cases the red and blue curves are nearly mirror images: 30% increase and 30% decrease in these two parameters produce inverse effects. The differences are somewhat smaller than those shown in Fig. 30, rising to about 3—4 dB.

Figure 31. Spectral differences in the same format as Fig. 30, for the case of a 30% reduction in rocking moment of inertia (red curve) and a 30% increase in rocking moment of inertia (blue curve).
Figure 32. Spectral differences in the same format as Fig. 30, for the case of a 30% reduction in effective rocking length (red curve) and a 30% increase in effective rocking length (blue curve).

Figures 33 and 34 show corresponding plots for variation of the two parameters that influence the bouncing mode of the bridge. The curves become rather noisy at higher frequency, but in the main the differences revealed here are quite small.

Figure 33. Spectral differences in the same format as Fig. 30, for the case of a 30% reduction in bouncing frequency (red curve) and a 30% increase in bouncing frequency (blue curve).
Figure 34. Spectral differences in the same format as Fig. 30, for the case of a 30% reduction in effective bouncing mass (red curve) and a 30% increase in effective bouncing mass (blue curve).

Violin makers often comment on the fact that some instruments seem to be more sensitive than others to bridge adjustment. Plots like Figs. 30—34 give one possible way to investigate this issue. Figure 35 shows similar plots for a different violin, an ultra-light instrument by Joseph Curtin. The datum case here is based on that violin with its own bridge, which is also very light. Variations were then investigated by increasing and decreasing the parameters by 30%, as in the previous plots. The top left panel shows the effect of varying the rocking frequency, and should be compared to Fig. 30. The other panels show the effects of rocking moment of inertia (to be compared to Fig. 31), effective rocking length (to be compared to Fig. 32), and bouncing frequency (to be compared to Fig. 33).

The two violins show similar general patterns for all four comparisons, but there are significant differences of detail. When the rocking frequency is varied, the predicted spectral changes are rather smaller for the second violin than for the first: up to 3—4 dB, compared to 5–6 dB. For variation of rocking moment of inertia and effective rocking length, though, the predicted effect is if anything slightly larger than before — and it is concentrated around a slightly higher frequency. For variation of the bouncing frequency the plot is much clearer for the Curtin violin, but the differences resulting from changing the bouncing frequency are still small.

E. Complications, complications…

In this section we have shown that a simplified model of a violin bridge can give reasonably good fits to measured admittances, and can then be used to investigate various issues relevant to bridge adjustment. The simplified model has captured the main elements of the underlying physics, and in the context of the other sections in this website, that is as much as we usually ask. However, violin makers have a particular interest in a quantitative understanding of the subtleties of bridge adjustment, and for their benefit it is worth giving a discussion of some further details: features missing from the model presented so far, and issues relating to possible workshop use of this kind of modelling. The general reader will probably not be interested, so the discussion is tucked away in the next side link.

SEE MORE DETAIL

NEXT SECTION